LANGUAGE IMPACT TOWARDS SOCIAL BEHAVIOR OF AT-RISK CHILDREN

Authors

  • Dr. Katerina Zlatkova-Doncheva St.Cyril and St. Methodius University of Veliko Tarnovo, Veliko Tarnovo, Republic of Bulgaria

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.20319/pijtel.2019.31.0121

Keywords:

Children at Risk, Children Deprived of Parental Care, Social Behavior, Language, Paralinguistic Signs, Lexical Content

Abstract

Current study examines the use of language towards behavior of at-risk children. At-risk youth living separated from their family are deemed as most vulnerable children with highest risk having high level of anxiety, emotional disorders, aggression and problematic behavior. Children deprived of parental care living across Bulgarian residential homes (N=40) divided into 3 age groups (aged 7-10; aged 11-14; and aged 15-17) participated in 12 experimental activities and has been subjected surveillance of changes in their behavior according to certain use of language and voice (tone). Children adhere to or violate the rules in activities was the main criteria that have been defined for observation. The use of language indicators were divided into certain words with encouraging and reprobation meaning combined with louder or normal voice. ANOVA measures of surveillance assessed the impact of language and speech on behavior of children and indicated significant results for different age groups. The main hypothesis of the survey traces the impact of encouraging words with positive meaning and normal voice for better effect on socially acceptable behavior. Scientifically justified findings of this study indicates existence of a pattern supported the positive impact of language and speech that specialists and teachers can use helping the child absorbing pro-social behavior and gives directions of an effective language-based model of consulting and educating children. The results in current research can help social workers, social educators, teachers and psychologists to find suitable strategy in communication and proper use of language for better results on socialization and adaptation of the child with behavior problems.

References

Brown-Schmidt S. (2009). The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychoomic Bulletin & Review, 16 (pp.893–900). doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.893 [PubMed][CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.5.893

Burgess P. W. (1997). Theory and methodology in executive function and research. Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function, Hove, UK: Psychology Press, (pp. 81–116).

Campos JJ, Barrett KC. (1984). Toward a new understanding of emotions and their development. Emotions, cognition, and behavior, Cambridge University Press, (pp. 229–263).

Clarkson MG, Clifton RK.(1985). Infant pitch perception: evidence for responding to pitch categories and the missing fundamental. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 1521–1528.[PubMed] https://doi.org/10.1121/1.391994

Fernald A.(1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64, (pp.657–674). [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.2307/1131209

Friend M, Becker J. (1987). Differences in the interpretation of discrepant and nondiscrepant messages by normal and disturbed children. Poster session presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; Baltimore, MD.

Friend, M. (2003). What Should I Do? Behavior Regulation by Language and Paralanguage in Early Childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development (vol.4, pp. 161–183). https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0402_02

Friend M. (2000).Developmental changes in sensitivity to vocal paralanguage. Developmental Science, 3 (pp.148–162) [PMC free article] [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00108

Friend M, Bryant JB. (2000). A developmental lexical bias in the interpretation of discrepant messages. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46 (pp.140–167). [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Gillis R., Nilsen E. S. (2014). The role of cognitive flexibility in children’s ability to detect communicative ambiguity. First Lanuage, 34, (pp. 58–71). doi: 10.1177/0142723714521839 [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723714521839

Golden M, Montare A, Bridger W. (1977). Verbal control of delay behavior in two-year-old boys as a function of social class. Child Development, 48, (pp. 1107–1111). https://doi.org/10.2307/1128370

Hancock JT, Dunham PJ, Purdy K. (2000). Children’s comprehension of critical and complimentary forms of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, (pp. 227–248). https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD010204

Huizinga M., Dolan C. V., van der Molen M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44 (pp.2017–2036), doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010 [PubMed] [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010

Kitamura, C, Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and Communicative Intent in Mother's Speech: Adjustments for Age and Sex in the First Year. Infancy, (vol. 4(1):85 – 110). https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0401_5

Kitamura, C., Lam, C. (2009). Age-Specific Preferences for Infant-Directed Affective Intent. Infancy, (vol.14, pp.77-100). ISSN: ISSN-1525-0008 https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000802569777

Luria AR. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. Tizard J, editor. New York: Liveright Publishing Corporation.

Milosky LM, Ford JA. (2000). The role of prosody in children’s inferences of ironic intent. Discourse Processes, 23, (pp. 47–61). https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539709544981

Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H., Howerter A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychology, 41, (pp. 49–100). doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [PubMed] [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Moore, C., Harris, L., Patriquin, M. (2008). Lexical and prosodic cues in the comprehension of relative certainty. Journal of Child Language, 20, (pp.153 – 167). doi: 10.1017/S030500090000917X https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000917X

Morton JB, Trehub SE. (2001). Children’s understanding of emotion in speech. Child Development, 72, 834–843. [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00318

Mumme DL, Fernald A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: Learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Development, 67(6), (pp. 3219-37). [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00532

Nilsen E. S., Graham S. A. (2012). The development of preschoolers’ appreciation of communicative ambiguity. Child Deelopment, 83, (pp.1400–1415), doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01762.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01762.x

Nilsen, E.S., Rints, A., Ethier, N., Moroz, S.(2016) Mother-Child Communication: The Influence of ADHD Symptomatology and Executive Functioning on Paralinguistic Style. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1203. doi: [10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01203] https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01203

Nilsen E. S., Varghese A., Fecica A., Xu Z. (2015). Children’s production of referential statements. An examination of the role of ADHD traits and executive functioning. J. Cogn. Dev., 36 , (pp.68–82). doi: 10.1017/S0305000911000432 [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000432

O’Neill DK. (2007). The language use inventory for young children: a parent-report measure of pragmatic language development for 18- to 47-month-old children. J. Speech Lang. Hear Res. 50214–228. DOI:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/017) [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/017)

Reilly SS, Muzekari LH. (1986). Effects of emotional illness and age upon the resolution of discrepant messages. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, (pp.823–829). [PubMed] https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.62.3.823

Saltz E, Campbell S, Skotko D. (1983). Verbal control of behavior: The effects of shouting. Developmental Psychology, 19 (pp. 461–464). https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.19.3.461

Solomon D, Ali FA. (1972). Age trends in the perception of verbal reinforcers. Developmental Psychology, 7 (pp.238–243). https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033354

Strommen EA. (1973).Verbal self-regulation in children’s games: Impulsive errors in “Simon says. Child Development, 44 (pp. 849–853). https://doi.org/10.2307/1127737

Struchen M. A., Clark A. N., Sander A. M., Mills M. R., Evans G., Kurtz D. (2008). Relation of executive functioning and social communication measures to functional outcomes following traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation, 23, (pp. 185–198). [PubMed]

Uekermann J., Abdel-Hamid M., Lehmkämper C., Vollmoeller W., Daum R. (2008). Perception of affective prosody in major depression: a link to executive functions? Journal of Inernational Neuropsychological Society, 14(4), (pp.552–561). doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080740 [PubMed] [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080740

Volkmar FR, Hoder EL, Siegel AE.(1980). Discrepant social communication. Developmental Psychology, 16, (pp. 495–505). https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.16.5.495

Wardlow L. (2013). Individual differences in speakers’ perspective taking: The roles of executive control and working memory. Psychoomic Bulletin & Review, 20, (766–772). doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0396-1 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0396-1

Георгиев, Г. (2016) Децата в конфликт със закона, като съвременна рискова група. В: Доклади от Международна научна конференция „Педагогическото образование – традиции и съвременност”, Издателство Ай анд Би, В.Търново, (с.37-42). ISSN: 2534-9317.

Димитрова, Я. (2016). Специфика на социалната работа с различни рискови групи. В: Доклади от Научна конференция „Социалното служение в съвременното общество” - Велико Търново: Либра –Скорп, с.189-196.

Златкова-Дончева, Катерина. (2016). Социалнопедагогическа характеристика на деца в риск, отглеждани извън семейна среда. Трета докторантска научна сесия: Сборник доклади / Велико Търново: Великотърновски университет „Св. Св. Кирил и Методий“, Педагогически факултет, с.55-70, ISBN 978-619-208-088-4

Колшанский, Г.В. (2009). Паралингвистика. Лингвистическое наследие XX века. ISBN 97805-484-01127-8

Кузманова-Карталова, Р. (2013). Социално-педагогическа работа с деца в риск. В. Търново: Университетско издателство „Св. Св. Кирил и Методий”.

Маринов, В. (2018) За интонационния контур на подчинените подложни изречения. Проблеми на устната комуникация, 11, с. 369 – 380.

Първанова,Й. (2003). Специфика на превантивната функция на домовете за отглеждане и възпитание на деца, лишени от родителска грижа. Превантивна педагогика като научно познание1 и 2 част София.

Downloads

Published

2019-03-15

How to Cite

Doncheva, K. Z. (2019). LANGUAGE IMPACT TOWARDS SOCIAL BEHAVIOR OF AT-RISK CHILDREN. PUPIL: International Journal of Teaching, Education and Learning, 3(1), 01–21. https://doi.org/10.20319/pijtel.2019.31.0121