THE COST OF MULTITASKING: A COMPUTER-ASSISTED QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF TASK-SWITCHING COSTS IN SPEED AND ACCURACY BY AGE AND GENDER

Authors

  • Joseph D. Lou Oxford Academy, Cypress, United States

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.20319/pijss.2018.43.323340

Keywords:

Multitasking, Task-Switching Cost, Age, Gender, Developmental Psychology

Abstract

Multitasking is known to have negative impacts on productivity. However, there has been no systematic study on its negative effects comparing age and gender groups at a large scale.  Due to limitations in previous methods, this study aimed to develop an original web-based Multitasking Test (MTT) tool and then use it to test a large sample of diverse participants on their task-switching costs (time and accuracy lost when multitasking).  MTT allowed participants (N = 1,004) to each conduct 6 pattern-matching tasks with playing cards (20 trials per task).  Results indicated that (a) high schoolers took 95% more time and made 120% more errors when multitasking than when performing single tasks separately; (b) the 22-25 age group performed the best among all groups, consistent with previous neuroscience findings that the human brain does not reach maturity until 24 years old; and (c) there were significant gender differences (p < .001) in switching costs in time, potentially explained by gender differences in the structural connectome of human brains.  This study presents an innovative, computer-assisted methodological design and demonstrates how multitasking has switching costs across all age and gender groups regardless of prior experiences and education level.  In addition to presenting strong empirical data that support the Cognitive Load Theory, it also provides a reliable multitasking paradigm for further studies in cognitive science, developmental psychology, and neuroscience.  

References

American Psychological Association. (2006). Multitasking: switching costs. Retrieved from the American Psychological Association website: http://www.apa.org/research/action/multitask.aspx

Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A., Sandhu, R. & Sharma, S. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 9, 449.

Czerwinski, M., Cutrell, E., & Horvitz, E. (2000). Instant messaging: Effects of relevance and timing. In People and computers XIV: Proceedings of HCI, 2, 71-76.

Fox, A. B., Rosen, J., & Crawford, M. (2009). Distractions, distractions: does instant messaging affect college students' performance on a concurrent reading comprehension task?. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(1), 51-53. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0107

Gaither, C. (2006). They do it all while studying. Los Angeles Times. Retrieved from http://articles.latimes.com/2006/aug/11/business/fi-pollhomework11

Grattan, D. R., & Akopian, A. N. (2016). Oscillating from neurosecretion to multitasking dopamine neurons. Cell reports, 15(4), 681-682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.013

Ingalhalikar, M., Smith, A., Parker, D., Satterthwaite, T. D., Elliott, M. A., Ruparel, K., Hakonarson, H., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., & Verma, R. (2014). Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 823-828. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1316909110

Kray, J., Eber, J., & Karbach, J. (2008). Verbal self‐instructions in task switching: a compensatory tool for action‐control deficits in childhood and old age?. Developmental science, 11(2), 223-236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00673.x

Kirschner, P.A. (2002). Cognitive load theory: Implications of cognitive load theory on the design of learning. Learning and Instruction, 12, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00014-7

Koch, I., Poljac, E., Müller, H., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Cognitive structure, flexibility, and plasticity in human multitasking – An integrative review of dual-task and task-switching research. Psychological bulletin, 144(6), 557. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000144

Lien, T. (2015, Nov. 3). Teens spend an average of 9 hours a day with media, survey finds. Los Angeles Times. Retrieved from http://www.latimes.com/business/technology/la-fi-tn-teens-television-media-20151103-story.html

Paas, F., van Gog, T., & Sweller, J. (2010). Cognitive load theory: New conceptualizations, specifications, and integrated research perspectives. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 115-121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9133-8

Rogers R.D., Monsell S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231 https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E. & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive Control of Cognitive Processes in Task Switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 763-797. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.763

Seereekissoon, D. S. (2018). Flipped classroom teaching in higher education: An assumption or an inevitable tool. PEOPLE: International Journal of Social Sciences, 4(1). https://dx.doi.org/10.20319/pijss.2018.41.494506

Strobach, T., Liepelt, R., Schubert, T., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Task switching: effects of practice on switch and mixing costs. Psychological Research, 76(1), 74-83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0323-x

Stoet, G., O’Connor, D. B., Conner, M., & Laws, K. R. (2013). Are women better than men at multi-tasking?. BMC Psychology, 1(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-7283-1-18

Tham, R., & Tham, L. (2015). Game-based learning in Singapore higher education – a pilot study. PEOPLE: International Journal of Social Sciences, 1(1). https://dx.doi.org/10.20319/pijss.2015.s21.571581

The Common Sense Media. (2015). The common sense census: Media use by tweens and teens. Retrieved from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/Census_researchreport.pdf

Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent?. Journal of memory and language, 28(2), 127-154. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90040-5

Walsh, D. (2011, July 9). Can kids multitask?. Psychology Today. Retrieved from http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/smart-parenting-smarter-kids/201107/can-kids-multitask

Wong, A. S., Cooper, P. S., Conley, A. C., McKewen, M., Fulham, W. R., Michie, P. T., & Karayanidis, F. (2018). Event-Related Potential Responses to Task Switching Are Sensitive to Choice of Spatial Filter. Frontiers in neuroscience, 12, 143. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00143

Downloads

Published

2018-11-22

How to Cite

Lou, J. D. (2018). THE COST OF MULTITASKING: A COMPUTER-ASSISTED QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF TASK-SWITCHING COSTS IN SPEED AND ACCURACY BY AGE AND GENDER. PEOPLE: International Journal of Social Sciences, 4(3), 323–340. https://doi.org/10.20319/pijss.2018.43.323340